Author Summary | The value and the availability of an option can change dynamically even during ongoing actions which compounds the decision-making challenge. |
Author Summary | It combines dynamic neural field theory with stochastic optimal control theory, and includes circuitry for perception, eXpected reward, effort cost and decision-making . |
Discussion | Classic serial theories, such as the “goods-based” model, suggest that decision-making is an independent cognitive process from the sensorimotor processes that implement the resulting choice [1]. |
Discussion | These findings suggest that sensorimotor and midbrain regions may be causally involved in the process of decision-making and action-selection. |
Discussion | For instance, studies in oculomotor decision-making suggest that LIP neurons accumulate the evidence associated with the alternative options and the accumulated activity of these neurons is compared by “decision brain areas” to select an option [13, 56]. |
Introduction | In such situations, the goals dynamically compete during movement, and it is not possible to clearly separate goal decision-making from action selection. |
Introduction | In the current study, we propose a distributed neurodynamical framework that models the neural basis of decision-making between actions. |
Introduction | It builds on successful models in dynamic neural field theory [25] and stochastic optimal control theory [26] and includes circuitry for perception, expected reward, selection bias, decision-making and effort cost. |
Visuomotor decisions with competing alternatives | We show how the proposed computational model can be extended to involve motor plan formation DNFs for various effectors which interact competitively to implement effector as well as spatial decision-making . |
Abstract | We discuss applications to experimental recordings in classic sensory decision-making tasks, which will hopefully provide new insights into the nature of perceptual integration. |
Author Summary | This article deals With the interpretation of neural activities during perceptual decision-making tasks, Where animals must assess the value of a sensory stimulus and take a decision on the basis of their percept. |
Discussion | We have proposed a framework to interpret sensitivity and choice signals in a standard model of perceptual decision-making . |
Experimental measures of behavior and neural activities | We will study the formation of percepts in the context of perceptual decision-making experiments (Fig. |
Experimental measures of behavior and neural activities | For concreteness, we will mostly focus on these discrimination tasks, although the general framework can be applied to arbitrary perceptual decision-making tasks. |
Experimental statistics of neural activity and choice | Classic measures in decision-making experiments can be interpreted as estimates of the first-and second-order statistics of choice c and recorded spike trains ri(t), across all trials with a fixed stimulus value 5: |
Introduction | These questions are central to our understanding of percept formation and decision-making in the brain and have been the focus of much previous work [1]. |
Introduction | Many studies have sought to address these problems by studying animals that perform simple, perceptual decision-making tasks [2, 3]. |
Abrupt and gradual shifts in reach direction as a consequence of optimal control under goal uncertainty | We combined this decision-making process with an optimal control model of movement generation. |
Abrupt and gradual shifts in reach direction as a consequence of optimal control under goal uncertainty | Paralleling standard models of decision-making [3,26], rt represents the log odds ratio of the belief pt that the initial target is the true target location: rt 2 log (11’; ). |
Abstract | Our results and theory suggest a more integrated view of decision-making and movement planning in which the primary bottleneck to generating a movement is deciding upon task goals. |
Implications for neural representations of movement planning and execufion | Indeed, the state of motor areas appears to continuously track belief state during decision-making tasks [49,50]. |
Limitations of the model | Additional sources of asymmetry may arise from intrinsic biases in subjects’ decision-making processes; subjects may be inherently biased against changing their minds [34]. |
Optimal action selection amid evolving uncertainty about task goals | As is commonly assumed in decision-making models [3,26], we model rt as following a Gaussian random walk: n+1 N N (rt, 03). |
Optimal action selection amid evolving uncertainty about task goals | In models of decision-making , the stochastic nature of rt reflects a distribution over stimuli that the subject may have perceived in a given trial. |
Abstract | Strikingly, it remains largely unclear how humans evaluate rewards when these are tied to energetic costs, despite the surge of interest in the neural basis of effort-guided decision-making and the prevalence of disorders showing a diminished willingness to exert effort (e.g., depression). |
Author Summary | Our model will be important for characterizing decision-making deficits in clinical disorders characterized by behavioral inactivity. |
Introduction | Understanding how different decision costs influence human reward discounting has obvious implications for economic decisions, and for clinical disorders, such as anxiety and impulsivity disorders, depression, gambling, and addiction [14—21], in which deficits in cost-benefit decision-making are hallmark features of the disease. |
Introduction | These dissociable psychological constructs are supported by both neurophysiological and neuropsychological data: lesions of anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)—an area implicated in de-cision-making and action selection which connects strongly to the motor system—cause deficits in effort-based, but not delay-based, choice behavior, while lesions to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)—an area implicated in reward processing but that does not connect with the motor system—cause deficits in delay-based, but not effort-based, decision-making [46]. |
Introduction | The hyperbolic effect of delay discounting has been used extensively as a model for self-con-trol and provides a powerful tool to quantify decision-making differences in the normal and clinical populations (e.g., impulsivity, addiction), and even as a predictor of financial mismanagement [47—56]. |
Author Summary | We show that sequential decision-making behavior cannot easily be predicted from the results of simple one-off choices made at the beginning of the task. |
Discussion | The prominent tendencies to either save relief or to spread relief across time here may have implications for dynamic health-related decision-making in the field. |
Discussion | Applied measures of choice over time have tended to focus exclusively on one-off choice paradigms [50—53], and the modelling of dynamic decision-making tasks suggests a novel and quantitatively rich behavioral predictor. |