Index of papers in PLOS Comp. Biol. that mention
  • effort costs
Miriam C. Klein-Flügge, Steven W. Kennerley, Ana C. Saraiva, Will D. Penny, Sven Bestmann
Abstract
Contrary to previous reports, in both experiments effort costs devalued reward in a manner opposite to delay, with small devaluations for lower efforts, and progressively larger devaluations for higher effort-levels (concave shape).
Effort discounting is concave and differs from delay discounting
More generally, data from two tasks in which effort costs were unconfounded from delay costs provide robust evidence against the notion that effort discounting is best explained by a hyperbolic function as previously suggested [29,38,39].
Effort discounting is concave and differs from delay discounting
Critically, we note that previous studies did not directly compare the performance of the hyperbolic or linear model to any alternative models, and did not dissociate choices involving delay and effort costs .
Experiment 1: Dissociating effort and delay discounting
Thus, effort costs were uncon-founded from delay costs.
Experiment 2: Indifference curves for effort discounting
This ensured that effort costs were unconfounded from delay costs.
Introduction
The most commonly applied models of effort discounting assume that effort costs decrease reward value either linearly [31,37] or hyperbolically [29,38,39] , and thus in the same way as delay (although see [40]).
Introduction
Indeed, several theoretical arguments suggest that individuals may incorporate effort costs into their decisions in a different way to delay costs.
Introduction
Finally, from a psychological perspective, it has been argued that the two costs differ fundamentally with regard to what the cost is ascribed to: effort costs are ascribed to actions, whereas delay costs (when not caused by movement times) are ascribed to outcomes [43, but see 44,45].
Results are not trivially explained by a larger number of model parameters, the exerted force, or fatigue
We conducted a control analysis to test whether this nonlinear variability increase in the produced force could explain the concave shape of discounting observed for effort costs .
Results are not trivially explained by a larger number of model parameters, the exerted force, or fatigue
Thus, effort discounting is concave independent of whether participant’s evaluation relies on the required or the predicted produced effort cost .
effort costs is mentioned in 17 sentences in this paper.
Topics mentioned in this paper:
Vassilios Christopoulos, James Bonaiuto, Richard A. Andersen
Author Summary
It combines dynamic neural field theory with stochastic optimal control theory, and includes circuitry for perception, eXpected reward, effort cost and decision-making.
Discussion
It is comprised of a series of dynamic neural fields (DNFs) that simulate the neural processes underlying motor plan formation, expected reward and effort cost .
Introduction
It builds on successful models in dynamic neural field theory [25] and stochastic optimal control theory [26] and includes circuitry for perception, expected reward, selection bias, decision-making and effort cost .
Model architecture
The basic architecture of the framework is a set of dynamic neural fields (DNFs) that capture the neural processes underlying cue perception, motor plan formation, valuation of goods (e.g., eXpected reward/punishment, social reward, selection bias, cognitive bias) and valuation of actions (e.g., effort cost , precision required), Fig.
effort costs is mentioned in 4 sentences in this paper.
Topics mentioned in this paper:
Adrian M. Haith, David M. Huberdeau, John W. Krakauer
Abrupt and gradual shifts in reach direction as a consequence of optimal control under goal uncertainty
Together with an effort cost I“, the overall expected endpoint cost is then given by a sum of the accuracy costs associated with each target, weighted by their beliefs (Equation 9).
Optimal action selection amid evolving uncertainty about task goals
In addition to this accuracy cost, we assume an effort cost Iu that penalizes large motor commands.
Optimal action selection amid evolving uncertainty about task goals
Following standard approaches [20], however, we assume that this effort cost is a quadratic function of the overall sequence of motor commands:
Optimal action selection amid evolving uncertainty about task goals
According to the optimal feedback control hypothesis [20], the motor system selects motor commands at that minimize the sum of accuracy and effort costs:
effort costs is mentioned in 4 sentences in this paper.
Topics mentioned in this paper: